|
Table of Contents
"TRUTH" DRUGS IN INTERROGATION
The search for effective aids to interrogation is probably as old as man's need to obtain
information from an uncooperative source and as persistent as his impatience to shortcut
any tortuous path. In the annals of police investigation, physical coercion has at times
been substituted for painstaking and time-consuming inquiry in the belief that direct
methods produce quick results. Sir James Stephens, writing in 1883, rationalizes a grisly
example of "third degree" practices by the police of India: "It is far
pleasanter to sit comfortably in the shade rubbing red pepper in a poor devil's eyes than
to go about in the sun hunting up evidence."
More recently, police officials in some countries have turned to drugs for assistance in
extracting confessions from accused persons, drugs which are presumed
-26-
to relax the individual's defenses to the point that he unknowingly reveals truths he has
been trying to conceal. This investigative technique, however humanitarian as an
alternative to physical torture, still raises serious questions of individual rights and
liberties. In this country, where drugs have gained only marginal acceptance in police
work, their use has provoked cries of "psychological third degree" and has
precipitated medico-legal controversies that after a quarter of a century still
occasionally flare into the open.
The use of so-called "truth" drugs in police work is similar to the accepted
psychiatric practice of narco-analysis; the difference in the two procedures lies in their
different objectives. The police investigator is concerned with empirical truth that may
be used against the suspect, and therefore almost solely with probative truth: the
usefulness of the suspect's revelations depends ultimately on their acceptance in evidence
by a court of law. The psychiatrist, on the other hand, using the same "truth"
drugs in diagnosis and treatment of the mentally ill, is primarily concerned with psychological
truth or psychological reality rather than empirical fact. A patient's aberrations are
reality for him at the time they occur, and an accurate account of these fantasies and
delusions, rather than reliable recollection of past events, can be the key to recovery.
The notion of drugs capable of illuminating hidden recesses of the mind, helping to heal
the mentally ill and preventing or reversing the miscarriage of justice, has provided an
exceedingly durable theme for the press and popular literature. While acknowledging that
"truth serum" is a misnomer twice over -- the drugs are not sera and they do not
necessarily bring forth probative truth -- journalistic accounts continue to exploit the
appeal of the term. The formula is to play up a few spectacular "truth" drug
successes and to imply that the drugs are more maligned than need be and more widely
employed in criminal investigation than can officially be admitted.
Any technique that promises an increment of success in extracting information from an
uncompliant source is ipso facto of interest in intelligence operations. If the
ethical considerations which in Western countries inhibit the use of narco-interrogation
in police work are felt also in intelligence, the Western services must at least be
prepared against its possible employment by the adversary. An understanding of
"truth" drugs, their characteristic actions, and their potentialities, positive
and negative, for eliciting useful information is fundamental to an adequate defense
against them.
This discussion, meant to help toward such an understanding, draws primarily upon openly
published materials. It has the limitations of projecting from criminal investigative
practices and from the permissive atmosphere of drug psychotherapy.
SCOPOLAMINE AS "TRUTH SERUM"
Early in this century physicians began to employ scopolamine, along with morphine and
chloroform, to induce a state of "twilight sleep" during childbirth. A
constituent of henbane, scopolamine was known to produce sedation and drowsiness,
confusion and disorientation, incoordination, and amnesia for events experienced during
intoxication. Yet physicians noted that women in twilight sleep answered questions
accurately and often volunteered exceedingly candid remarks.
In 1922 it occurred to Robert House, a Dallas, Texas obstetrician, that a similar
technique might be employed in the interrogation of suspected criminals, and he arranged
to interview under scopolamine two prisoners in the Dallas county jail whose guilt seemed
clearly confirmed. Under the drug, both men denied the charges on which they were held;
and both, upon trial, were found not guilty. Enthusiastic at this success, House concluded
that a patient under the influence of scopolamine "cannot create a lie... and there
is no power to think or reason." [14] His
experiment and this conclusion attracted wide attention, and the idea of a
"truth" drug was thus launched upon the public consciousness.
The phrase "truth serum" is believed to have appeared first in a news report of
House's experiment in the Los Angeles Record, sometime in 1922. House resisted the
term for a while but eventually came to employ it regularly himself. He published some
eleven articles on scopolamine in the years 1921-1929, with a noticeable increase in
polemical zeal as time when on. What had begun as something of a scientific statement
turned finally into a dedicated crusade by the "father of truth serum" on behalf
of his offspring, wherein he was "grossly indulgent of its wayward behavior and
stubbornly proud of its minor achievements." [11]
-27-
Only a handful of cases in which scopolamine was used for police interrogation came to
public notice, though there is evidence suggesting that some police forces may have used
it extensively. [2,16]
One police writer claims that the threat of scopolamine interrogation has been
effective in extracting confessions from criminal suspects, who are told they will first
be rendered unconscious by chloral hydrate placed covertly in their coffee or drinking
water. [16]
Because of a number of undesirable side effects, scopolamine was shortly disqualified as a
"truth" drug. Among the most disabling of the side effects are hallucinations,
disturbed perception, somnolence, and physiological phenomena such as headache, rapid
heart, and blurred vision, which distract the subject from the central purpose of the
interview. Furthermore, the physical action is long, far outlasting the psychological
effects. Scopolamine continues, in some cases, to make anesthesia and surgery safer by
drying the mouth and throat and reducing secretions that might obstruct the air passages.
But the fantastically, almost painfully, dry "desert" mouth brought on by the
drug is hardly conducive to free talking, even in a tractable subject.
THE BARBITURATES
The first suggestion that drugs might facilitate communication with emotionally disturbed
patients came quite by accident in 1916. Arthur S. Lovenhart and his associates at the
University of Wisconsin, experimenting with respiratory stimulants, were surprised when,
after an injection of sodium cyanide, a catatonic patient who had long been mute and rigid
suddenly relaxed, opened his eyes, and even answered a few questions. By the early 1930's
a number of psychiatrists were experimenting with drugs as an adjunct to established
methods of therapy.
At about this time police officials, still attracted by the possibility that drugs might
help in the interrogation of suspects and witnesses, turned to a class of depressant drugs
known as the barbiturates. By 1935 Clarence W. Muehlberger, head of the Michigan Crime
Detection Laboratory at East Lansing, was using barbiturates on reluctant suspects, though
police work continued to be hampered by the courts' rejection of drug-induced confessions
except in a few carefully circumscribed instances.
The barbiturates, first synthesized in 1903, are among the oldest of modern drugs and the
most versatile of all depressants. In this half-century some 2,500 have been prepared, and
about two dozen of these have won an important place in medicine. An estimated three to
four billion doses of barbiturates are prescribed by physicians in the United States each
year, and they have come to be known by a variety of commercial names and colorful slang
expressions: "goofballs," Luminal, Nembutal, "red devils,"
"yellow jackets," "pink ladies," etc. Three of them which are used in
narcoanalysis and have seen service as "truth" drugs are sodium amytal
(anobarbital), pentothal sodium (thiopental), and to a lesser extent seconal
(seconbarbital).
As one pharmacologist explains it, a subject coming under the influence of a barbiturate
injected intravenously goes through all the stages of progressive drunkenness, but the
time scale is on the order of minutes instead of hours. Outwardly the sedation effect is
dramatic, especially if the subject is a psychiatric patient in tension. His features
slacken, his body relaxes. Some people are momentarily excited; a few become silly and
giggly. This usually passes, and most subjects fall asleep, emerging later in disoriented
semi-wakefulness.
The descent into narcosis and beyond with progressively larger doses can be divided as
follows:
I. Sedative stage.
II. Unconsciousness, with exaggerated
reflexes (hyperactive stage).
III. Unconsciousness, without reflex even
to painful stimuli.
IV. Death.
Whether all these stages can be distinguished in any given subject depends largely on the
dose and the rapidity with which the drug is induced. In anesthesia, stages I and II may
last only two or three seconds.
The first or sedative stage can be further divided:
Plane 1. No evident effect, or slightly
sedative effect.
Plane 2. Cloudiness, calmness, amnesia.
(Upon recovery, the subject will not remember what happened at this or "lower"
planes or stages.)
Plane 3. Slurred speech, old thought
patterns disrupted, inability to integrate or learn new patterns. Poor coordination.
Subject becomes unaware of painful stimuli.
-28-
Plane 3 is the psychiatric "work" stage. It may last only a few minutes, but it
can be extended by further slow injection of drug. The usual practice is to back into the
sedative stage on the way to full consciousness.
CLINICAL AND EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES
The general abhorrence in Western countries for the use of chemical agents "to make
people do things against their will" has precluded serious systematic study (at least
as published openly) of the potentialities of drugs for interrogation. Louis A.
Gottschalk, surveying their use in information-seeking interviews, [13] cites 136 references; but only two touch upon the
extraction of intelligence information, and one of these concludes merely that Russian
techniques in interrogation and indoctrination are derived from age-old police methods and
do not depend on the use of drugs. On the validity of confessions obtained with
drugs, Gottschalk found only three published experimental studies that he deemed worth
reporting.
One of these reported experiments by D.P. Morris in which intravenous sodium amytal was
helpful in detecting malingerers. [12] The subjects,
soldiers, were at first sullen, negativistic, and non-productive under amytal, but as the
interview proceeded they revealed the fact of and causes for their malingering. Usually
the interviews turned up a neurotic or psychotic basis for the deception.
The other two confession studies, being more relevant to the highly specialized, untouched
area of drugs in intelligence interrogation, deserve more detailed review.
Gerson and Victoroff [12] conducted amytal interviews with
17 neuropsychiatric patients, soldiers who had charges against them, at Tilton General
Hospital, Fort Dix. First they were interviewed without amytal by a psychiatrist, who,
neither ignoring nor stressing their situation as prisoners or suspects under scrutiny,
urged each of them to discuss his social and family background, his army career, and his
version of the charges pending against him.
The patients were told only a few minutes in advance that narcoanalysis would be
performed. The doctor was considerate, but positive and forthright. He indicated that they
had no choice but to submit to the procedure. Their attitudes varied from unquestioning to
downright refusal.
Each patient was brought to complete narcosis and permitted to sleep. As he became
semiconscious and could be stimulated to speak, he was held in this stage with additional
amytal while the questioning proceeded. He was questioned first about innocuous matters
from his background that he had discussed before receiving the drug. Whenever possible, he
was manipulated into bringing up himself the charges pending against him before being
questioned about them. If he did this in a too fully conscious state, it proved more
effective to ask him to "talk about that later" and to interpose a topic that
would diminish suspicion, delaying the interrogation on his criminal activity until he was
back in the proper stage of narcosis.
The procedure differed from therapeutic narcoanalysis in several ways: the setting, the
type of patients, and the kind of "truth" sought. Also, the subjects were kept
in twilight consciousness longer than usual. This state proved richest in yield of
admissions prejudicial to the subject. In it his speech was thick, mumbling, and
disconnected, but his discretion was markedly reduced. This valuable interrogation period,
lasting only five to ten minutes at a time, could be reinduced by injecting more amytal
and putting the patient back to sleep.
The interrogation technique varied from case to case according to the background
information about the patient, the seriousness of the charges, the patient's attitude
under narcosis, and his rapport with the doctor. Sometimes it was useful to pretend, as
the patient grew more fully conscious, that he had already confessed during the amnestic
period of the interrogation, and to urge him, while his memory and sense of
self-protection were still limited, to continue to elaborate the details of what he had
"already described." When it was obvious that a subject was withholding the
truth, his denials were quickly passed over and ignored, and the key questions would be
rewarded in a new approach.
Several patients revealed fantasies, fears, and delusions approaching delirium, much of
which could readily be distinguished from reality. But sometimes there was no way for the
examiner to distinguish truth from fantasy except by reference to other sources. One
subject claimed to have a child that did not exist,
-29-
another threatened to kill on sight a stepfather who had been dead a year, and yet another
confessed to participating in a robbery when in fact he had only purchased goods from the
participants. Testimony concerning dates and specific places was untrustworthy and often
contradictory because of the patient's loss of time-sense. His veracity in citing names
and events proved questionable. Because of his confusion about actual events and what he
thought or feared had happened, the patient at times managed to conceal the truth
unintentionally.
As the subject revived, he would become aware that he was being questioned about his
secrets and, depending upon his personality, his fear of discovery, or the degree of his
disillusionment with the doctor, grow negativistic, hostile, or physically aggressive.
Occasionally patients had to be forcibly restrained during this period to prevent injury
to themselves or others as the doctor continued to interrogate. Some patients, moved by
fierce and diffuse anger, the assumption that they had already been tricked into
confessing, and a still limited sense of discretion, defiantly acknowledged their guilt
and challenged the observer to "do something about it." As the excitement
passed, some fell back on their original stories and others verified the confessed
material. During the follow-up interview nine of the 17 admitted the validity of their
confessions; eight repudiated their confessions and reaffirmed their earlier accounts.
With respect to the reliability of the results of such interrogation, Gerson and Victoroff
conclude that persistent, careful questioning can reduce ambiguities in drug
interrogation, but cannot eliminate them altogether.
At least one experiment has shown that subjects are capable of maintaining a lie while
under the influence of a barbiturate. Redlich and his associates at Yale [25] administered sodium amytal to nine volunteers, students
and professionals, who had previously, for purposes of the experiment, revealed shameful
and guilt-producing episodes of their past and then invented false self-protective stories
to cover them. In nearly every case the cover story retained some elements of the guilt
inherent in the true story.
Under the influence of the drug, the subjects were crossexamined on their cover stories by
a second investigator. The results, though not definitive, showed that normal individuals
who had good defenses and no overt pathological traits could stick to their invented
stories and refuse confession. Neurotic individuals with strong unconscious self-punitive
tendencies, on the other hand, both confessed more easily and were inclined to substitute
fantasy for the truth, confessing to offenses never actually committed.
In recent years drug therapy has made some use of stimulants, most notably amphetamine
(Benzedrine) and its relative methamphetamine (Methadrine). These drugs, used either alone
or following intravenous barbiturates, produce an outpouring of ideas, emotions, and
memories which has been of help in diagnosing mental disorders. The potential of
stimulants in interrogation has received little attention, unless in unpublished work. In
one study of their psychiatric use Brussel et al. [7]
maintain that methedrine gives the liar no time to think or to organize his deceptions.
Once the drug takes hold, they say, an insurmountable urge to pour out speech traps the
malingerer. Gottschalk, on the other hand, says that this claim is extravagant, asserting
without elaboration that the study lacked proper controls. [13]
It is evident that the combined use of barbiturates and stimulants, perhaps along with
ataraxics (tranquilizers), should be further explored.
OBSERVATIONS FROM PRACTICE
J.M. MacDonald, who as a psychiatrist for the District Courts of Denver has had extensive
experience with narcoanalysis, says that drug interrogation is of doubtful value in
obtaining confessions to crimes. Criminal suspects under the influence of barbiturates may
deliberately withhold information, persist in giving untruthful answers, or falsely
confess to crimes they did not commit. The psychopathic personality, in particular,
appears to resist successfully the influence of drugs.
MacDonald tells of a criminal psychopath who, having agreed to narco-interrogation,
received 1.5 grams of sodium amytal over a period of five hours. This man feigned amnesia
and gave a false account of a murder. "He displayed little or no remorse as he
(falsely) described the crime, including burial of the body. Indeed he was very
self-possessed and he appeared almost to enjoy the examination. From time to time he would
request that more amytal be injected." [21]
MacDonald concludes that a person who gives false information prior to re-
-30-
ceiving drugs is likely to give false information also under narcosis, that the drugs are
of little value for revealing deceptions, and that they are more effective in releasing
unconsciously repressed material than in evoking consciously suppressed information.
Another psychiatrist known for his work with criminals, L.Z. Freedman, gave sodium amytal
to men accused of various civil and military antisocial acts. The subjects were mentally
unstable, their conditions ranging from character disorders to neuroses and psychoses. The
drug interviews proved psychiatrically beneficial to the patients, but Freedman found that
his view of objective reality was seldom improved by their revelations. He was unable to
say on the basis of the narco-interrogation whether a given act had or had not occurred.
Like MacDonald, he found that psychopathic individuals can deny to the point of
unconsciousness crimes that every objective sign indicates they have committed. [10]
F.G. Inbau, Professor of Law at Northwestern University, who has had considerable
experience observing and participating in "truth" drug tests, claims that they
are occasionally effective on persons who would have disclosed the truth anyway had they
been properly interrogated, but that a person determined to lie will usually be able to
continue the deception under drugs.
The two military psychiatrists who made the most extensive use of narcoanalysis during the
war years. Roy R. Grinker and John C. Spiegel, concluded that in almost all cases they
could obtain from their patients essentially the same material and give them the same
emotional release by therapy without the use of drugs, provided they had sufficient time.
The essence of these comments from professionals of long experience is that drugs provide
rapid access to information that is psychiatrically useful but of doubtful validity as
empirical truth. The same psychological information and a less adulterated empirical truth
can be obtained from fully conscious subjects through non-drug psychotherapy and skillful
police interrogation.
APPLICATION TO CI INTERROGATION
The almost total absence of controlled experimental studies of "truth" drugs and
the spotty and anecdotal nature of psychiatric and police evidence require that
extrapolations to intelligence operations be made with care. Still, enough is known about
the drugs' actions to suggest certain considerations affecting the possibilities for their
use in interrogation.
It should be clear from the foregoing that at best a drug can only serve as an aid to an
interrogator who has a sure understanding of the psychology and techniques of normal
interrogation. In some respects, indeed, the demands on his skill will be increased by the
baffling mixture of truth and fantasy in drug-induced output. And the tendency against
which he must guard in the interrogate to give the responses that seem to be wanted
without regard for facts will be heightened by drugs: the literature abounds with warnings
that a subject in narcosis is extremely suggestible.
It seems possible that this suggestibility and the lowered guard of the narcotic state
might be put to advantage in the case of a subject feigning ignorance of a language or
some other skill that had become automatic with him. Lipton [20]
found sodium amytal helpful in determining whether a foreign subject was merely pretending
not to understand English. By extension, one can guess that a drugged interrogatee might
have difficulty maintaining the pretense that he did not comprehend the idiom of a
profession he was trying to hide.
There is the further problem of hostility in the interrogator's relationship to a
resistance source. The accumulated knowledge about "truth" drug reaction has
come largely from patient-physician relationships of trust and confidence. The subject in
narcoanalysis is usually motivated a priori to cooperate with the psychiatrist, either to
obtain relief from mental suffering or to contribute to a scientific study. Even in police
work, where an atmosphere of anxiety and threat may be dominant, a relationship of trust
frequently asserts itself: the drug is administered by a medical man bound by a strict
code of ethics; the suspect agreeing to undergo narcoanalysis in a desperate bid for
corroboration of his testimony trusts both drug and psychiatrist, however apprehensively;
and finally, as Freedman and MacDonald have indicated, the police psychiatrist frequently
deals with a "sick" criminal, and some order of patient-physician relationship
necessarily evolves.
-31-
Rarely has a drug interrogation involved "normal" individuals in a hostile or
genuinely threatening milieu. It was from a non-threatening experimental setting that Eric
Lindemann could say that his "normal" subjects "reported a general sense of
euphoria, ease and confidence, and they exhibited a marked increase in talkativeness and
communicability." [18] Gerson and Victoroff list
poor doctor-patient rapport as one factor interfering with the completeness and
authenticity of confessions by the Fort Dix soldiers, caught as they were in a command
performance and told they had no choice but to submit to narco-interrogation.
From all indications, subject-interrogation rapport is usually crucial to obtaining the
psychological release which may lead to unguarded disclosures. Role-playing on the part of
the interrogator might be a possible solution to the problem of establishing rapport with
a drugged subject. In therapy, the British narco-analyst William Sargent recommends that
the therapist deliberately distort the facts of the patient's life-experience to achieve
heightened emotional response and abreaction. [27] In the
drunken state of narcoanalysis patients are prone to accept the therapist's false
constructions. There is reason to expect that a drugged subject would communicate freely
with an interrogator playing the role of relative, colleague, physician, immediate
superior, or any other person to whom his background indicated he would be responsive.
Even when rapport is poor, however, there remains one facet of drug action eminently
exploitable in interrogation -- the fact that subjects emerge from narcosis feeling they
have revealed a great deal, even when they have not. As Gerson and Victoroff demonstrated
at Fort Dix, this psychological set provides a major opening for obtaining genuine
confessions.
POSSIBLE VARIATIONS
In studies by Beecher and his associates, [3-6] one-third
to one-half the individuals tested proved to be placebo reactors, subjects who respond
with symptomatic relief to the administration of any syringe, pill, or capsule, regardless
of what it contains. Although no studies are known to have been made of the placebo
phenomenon as applied to narco-interrogation, it seems reasonable that when a subject's
sense of guilt interferes with productive interrogation, a placebo for pseudo-narcosis
could have the effect of absolving him of the responsibility for his acts and thus clear
the way for free communication. It is notable that placebos are most likely to be
effective in situations of stress. The individuals most likely to react to placebos are
the more anxious, more self-centered, more dependent on outside stimulation, those who
express their needs more freely socially, talkers who drain off anxiety by conversing with
others. The non-reactors are those clinically more rigid and with better than average
emotional control. No sex or I.Q. differences between reactors and non-reactors have been
found.
Another possibility might be the combined use of drugs with hypnotic trance and
post-hypnotic suggestion: hypnosis could presumably prevent any recollection of the drug
experience. Whether a subject can be brought to trance against his will or unaware,
however, is a matter of some disagreement. Orne, in a survey of the potential uses of
hypnosis in interrogation, [23] asserts that it is
doubtful, despite many apparent indications to the contrary, that trance can be induced in
resistant subjects. It may be possible, he adds, to hypnotize a subject unaware, but this
would require a positive relationship with the hypnotist not likely to be found in the
interrogation setting.
In medical hypnosis, pentothal sodium is sometimes employed when only light trance has
been induced and deeper narcosis is desired. This procedure is a possibility for
interrogation, but if a satisfactory level of narcosis could be achieved through hypnotic
trance there would appear to be no need for drugs.
DEFENSIVE MEASURES
There is no known way of building tolerance for a "truth" drug without creating
a disabling addiction, or of arresting the action of a barbiturate once induced. The only
full safeguard against narco-interrogation is to prevent the administration of the drug.
Short of this, the best defense is to make use of the same knowledge that suggests drugs
for offensive operations: if a subject knows that on emerging from narcosis he will have
an exaggerated notion of how much he has revealed he can better resolve to deny he has
said anything.
-32-
The disadvantages and shortcomings of drugs in offensive operations become positive
features of the defensive posture. A subject in narco-interrogation is garbled and
irrational, the amount of output drastically diminished. Drugs disrupt established thought
patterns, including the will to resist, but they do so indiscriminately and thus also
interfere with the patterns of substantive information the interrogator seeks. Even under
the conditions most favorable for the interrogator, output will be contaminated by
fantasy, distortion, and untruth.
Possibly the most effective way to arm oneself against narco-interrogation would be to
undergo a "dry run." A trial drug interrogation with output taped for playback
would familiarize an individual with his own reactions to "truth" drugs, and
this familiarity would help to reduce the effects of harassment by the interrogator before
and after the drug has been administered. From the viewpoint of the intelligence service,
the trial exposure of a particular operative to drugs might provide a rough benchmark for
assessing the kind and amount of information he would divulge in narcosis.
There may be concern over the possibility of drug addiction intentionally or accidentally
induced by an adversary service. Most drugs will cause addiction with prolonged use, and
the barbiturates are no exception. In recent studies at the U.S. Public Health Service
Hospital for addicts in Lexington, Ky., subjects received large doses of barbiturates over
a period of months. Upon removal of the drug, they experienced acute withdrawal symptoms
and behaved in every respect like chronic alcoholics.
Because their action is extremely short, however, and because there is little likelihood
that they would be administered regularly over a prolonged period, barbiturate
"truth" drugs present slight risk of operational addiction. If the adversary
service were intent on creating addiction in order to exploit withdrawal, it would have
other, more rapid means of producing states as unpleasant as withdrawal symptoms.
The hallucinatory and psychotomimetic drugs such as mescaline, marihuana, LSD-25, and
microtine are sometimes mistakenly associated with narcoanalytic interrogation. These
drugs distort the perception and interpretation of the sensory input to the central
nervous system and affect vision, audition, smell, the sensation of the size of body parts
and their position in space, etc. Mescaline and LSD-25 have been used to create
experimental "psychotic states," and in a minor way as aids in psychotherapy.
Since information obtained from a person in a psychotic drug state would be unrealistic,
bizarre, and extremely difficult to assess, the self-administration of LSD-25, which is
effective in minute dosages, might in special circumstances offer an operative temporary
protection against interrogation. Conceivably, on the other hand, an adversary service
could use such drugs to produce anxiety or terror in medically unsophisticated subjects
unable to distinguish drug-induced psychosis from actual insanity. An enlightened
operative could not be thus frightened, however, knowing that the effect of these
hallucinogenic agents is transient in normal individuals.
Most broadly, there is evidence that drugs have least effect on well-adjusted individuals
with good defenses and good emotional control, and that anyone who can withstand the
stress of competent interrogation in the waking state can do so in narcosis. The essential
resources for resistance thus appear to lie within the individual.
CONCLUSIONS
The salient points that emerge from this discussion are the following. No such magic brew
as the popular notion of truth serum exists. The barbiturates, by disrupting defensive
patterns, may sometimes be helpful in interrogation, but even under the best conditions
they will elicit an output contaminated by deception, fantasy, garbled speech, etc. A
major vulnerability they produce in the subject is a tendency to believe he has revealed
more than he has. It is possible, however, for both normal individuals and psychopaths to
resist drug interrogation; it seems likely that any individual who can withstand ordinary
intensive interrogation can hold out in narcosis. The best aid to a defense against
narco-interrogation is foreknowledge of the process and its limitations. There is an acute
need for controlled experimental studies of drug reaction, not only to depressants but
also to stimulants and to combinations of depressants, stimulants, and ataraxics.
-33-
REFERENCES
1. Adams, E. Barbiturates. Sci. Am., Jan. 1958, 198 (1),
60-64
2. Barkham, J. Truth Drugs: The new crime solver. Coronet, Jan.
1951, 29, 72-76.
3. Beecher, H. K. Anesthesia. Sci. Am., Jan. 1957, 198, p.
70.
4. -----. Appraisal of drugs intended to alter subjective responses,
symptoms. J. Amer. Med. Assn., 1955, 158, 399-401.
5. -----. Evidence for increased effectiveness of placebos with
increased stress. Amer. J. Physiol., 1956, 187, 163-169.
6. -----. Experimental pharmacology and measurement of the subjective
response. Science, 1953, 116. 157-162.
7. Brussel, J. A., Wilson, D. C., Jr., & Shankel, L. W. The use of
methedrine in psychiatric practice. Psychiat. Quart., 1954, 28, 381-394.
8. Delay, J. Pharmacologic explorations of the personality:
narcoanalysis and "methedrine" shock. Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 1949, 42,
492-496.
9. deRopp, R. S. Drugs and the Mind. New York: Grove Press, Inc.,
1960.
10. Freedman, L. Z. "Truth" drugs. Sci. Am., March
1960. 145-154.
11. Geis, G. In scopolamine veritas. The early history of drug-induced
statements. J. of Crim. Law., Criminal, & Pol. Sci., Nov.-Dec. 1959, 50
(4), 347-358.
12. Gerson, M. J. & Victoroff, V. Experimental investigation into
the validity of confessions obtained under sodium amytal narcosis. J. Clin. and Exp.
Psychopath., 1948, 9, 359-375.
13. Gottschalk, L. A. The use of drugs in information-seeking
interviews. Technical report #2, ARDC Study SR 177-D Contract AF 18 (600) 1797.
Dec. 1958. Bureau of Social Science Research, Inc.
14. House, R. E. The use of scopolamine in criminology. Texas St. J.
of Med., 1922, 18, 259.
15. Houston, F. A preliminary investigation into abreaction comparing
methedrine and sodium amytal with other methods. J. Ment. Sci., 1952, 98,
707-710.
16. Inbau, F. G. Self-incrimination. Springfield: C. C. Thomas,
1950.
17. Kidd, W. R. Police interrogation. 1940.
18. Legal dose of truth. Newsweek, Feb. 23, 1959, 28.
19. Lindemann, E. Psychological changes in normal and abnormal
individuals under the influence of sodium amytal. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1932, 11,
1083-1091.
20. Lipton, E. L. The amytal interview. A review. Amer. Practit.
Digest Treatm., 1950, 1, 148-163.
21. MacDonald, J. M. Narcoanalysis and criminal law. Amer. J.
Psychiat., 1954, 111, 283-288.
22. Morris, D. P. Intravenous barbiturates: an aid in the diagnosis and
treatment of conversion hysteria and malingering. Mil. Surg., 1945, 96, 509-513.
23. Orne, M. T. The potential uses of hypnosis in interrogation. An
evaluation. ARDC Study SR 177-D Contract AF 18 (600) 1797, Dec. 1958. Bureau of
Social Science Research, Inc.
24. Pelikan, E. W., & Kensler, C. J. Sedatives: Their pharmacalogy
and uses. Reprint from The Medical Clinics of North America. W. B. Saunders
Company, Sept. 1958.
25. Redlich, F. C., Ravitz, L. J., & Dression, G. H. Narcoanalysis
and truth. Amer. J. Psychiat., 1951, 107, 586-593.
26. Rolin, J. Police Drugs. Translated by L. J. Bendit. New York:
Philosophical Library, 1956.
27. Sargant, W., & Slater, E. Physical methods of treatment in
psychiatry. (3rd. ed.) Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1954.
28. Snider, R. S. Cerebellum. Sci. Am., Aug. 1958, 84.
29. Uhr, L., & Miller, L. G. (eds.). Drugs and Behavior. New
York-London: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1960.
Next: CIA Director
Stansfield Turners Testimony, Continued
Previous: Opening Testimony
of CIA Director Stansfield Turner
|